The Neuroscience of Illusions and Dictionaries 72
Scientific American is running a pair of stories about what words and illusions can tell us about the brain. Mark Changizi of the Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute is interviewed about his research into the relationship between the mechanisms of the brain and the evolution of language. The second article contains a slide-show of various illusions and why the brain interprets them as it does.
Re: (Score:2, Insightful)
As interesting as the idea (of developing visual acuity and color sensitivity) is, TFA teases us. It mentions that we developed a sensitivity to red because of the blushing mechanism (never mind that people of color probably do not blush like whites), but doesn't give us much more. A slideshow of optical illusions? Whee.
That being said, it's a compelling idea, but it really belongs in the realm of science fiction and historical fantasy.
The letter "Y" looks like it doe
Re: (Score:2)
Summary! (Score:1)
Link to the meat (Score:5, Informative)
Also, a summary of the illusion article: The brain uses context, rather than absolute sampling.
Re: (Score:2)
So can somebody explain? (Score:4, Interesting)
Re: (Score:1, Funny)
Re: (Score:2, Informative)
Re: (Score:1, Interesting)
Re: (Score:2)
Re:So can somebody explain? (Score:5, Informative)
Re:So can somebody explain? (Score:4, Insightful)
Re: (Score:2, Informative)
Re:So can somebody explain? (Score:4, Funny)
Re: (Score:2)
Re: (Score:2, Insightful)
Re: (Score:2)
For me, these new LED tailights are really distracting, as I see them flickering as they go past. I also find push-bike LED tailights that flash patterns annoying as hell.
I don't have any flicker problems with LED torches or traffic signals, just the rear LED's on cars and trucks.
Re: (Score:1)
LEDs turn on and off very quickly.
The jiggle you observe is the inteference pattern between these two phenomena.
Binocular vision and elephants (Score:5, Interesting)
I had always wondered why elephants had forward-facing eyes, since they are not predators... and this helps explain it. I had always supposed that it was because they were social animals, and communication ability and multiple individuals scanning for threats was better than one individual with a larger field of view. This makes even more sense if the scanning in a smaller area is more effective due to the binocular vision associated with forward-facing eyes.
Re: (Score:2)
A SIGHT TO BEHOLD
Re: (Score:3, Insightful)
If we didn't have forward facing eyes, just how effective would our hands be? We'd have lots of trouble grabbing things, because we'd have a very hard time judging the proper distance. Same with manipulating things.
Granted, elephants aren't exactly known for having hands, but they do use their trunk for a LOT of gripping and manipulating. How much trouble would they have, if they didn't have forward facing eyes?
Re: (Score:2, Insightful)
Rods and cones (Score:5, Insightful)
It seems to me that a more reasonable hypothesis is that trichromatic color vision co-evolved along with the colorings of fruits that primates would find nutritious. Emotional cues seem like a more subtle issue - as well as a mostly-solved problem - that would have taken advantage of color vision that was already partially or fully evolved.
Re: (Score:2)
But detecting fruit colors never was a survival requirement for animals that have it as its main food.
Still, i agree that it shouldnt be the only factor there. If seeing changes in body heat was just what was ne
Subtle flaw in your argument (Score:1, Interesting)
Let us grant
Re: (Score:3, Interesting)
More/Better Optical Illusions (Score:5, Interesting)
Maybe I missed something, but I found the second article to be a let-down.
(Warning for epileptics: if visual stimuli can set off a seizure for you, you should probably stay away from the following links. I am not susceptible, but I found the second link to be visually overwhelming at first.)
IMHO, more interesting galleries of examples can be found at Wikipedia's Optical Illusions [wikipedia.org] page and at Michael Bach's 78 Optical Illusions & Visual Phenomena [michaelbach.de] page.
Re: (Score:1)
Another Great Resource (Score:3, Interesting)
http://illusioncontest.neuralcorrelate.com/ [neuralcorrelate.com]
These are the newest and most interesting illusions that are found every year. Some of them are very interesting and quite clever.
Re: (Score:2, Interesting)
You must have done something wrong. I opened the image in Photoshop and used the eye dropper to sample the pixels. The A and B squares are the same.
Re: (Score:2)
Maybe your printer or printer driver is.
I opened the picture in Gimp and selected a region in square B. It's really weird, it seems to get darker and lighter as I move the selection around.
Re:Brightness and Color Illusions (Score:4, Interesting)
Fascinating really!
Difficulty RTFA (Score:2, Informative)
LiquidGeneration has another slideshow (Score:2)
Serious conceptual flaws (Score:5, Insightful)
Quoting from the slide show link:
The whole philosophy of perception that this quote embodies is fundamentally wrong. As an example of this, take a look at the first so-called "illusion" in the slideshow: the Edward Adelson checkerboard-and-shadon example [sciam.com]. This is called an "illusion" on the basis that our eyesight "misleads us" by telling us that a light square in the shadow is lighter than a dark one in the light, whereas they are, supposedly, "the same color." By "the same color," what they seem to mean is that the stimulus, i.e., the rays of light reflected or emitted from the squares that hit our retina, have the same spectrum and intensity.
What they're missing is that the point of vision, and perception in general, isn't to give us information about the rays of light that hit the retina. What vision does is give us information about the objects in our environment, which reflect or emit rays of light. The reason we see the two squares as having different colors, despite the fact that our retinas are getting the exact same pointwise stimulus from them, is because the visual system, using contextual information about light and shadow across the whole scene, can figure out that the surface spectral reflectivity of the two squares must be different. Square B looks lighter than square A because the visual system judges, correctly, that it must reflect more light. Or put alternatively: the visual system figures out that if the two squares were in the same light, the point stimulus from the reflected light rays would be different.
This is accurately reproducing an aspect of the physical reality of the outside world; vision is accurately reproducing the spectral reflectivity of surfaces in our environment, at the apparent expense of failing to reproduce the spectral distribution of the rays of light that hit our retina. But of course, the answer to that one is that the rays of light aren't the object of visual perception, they're just the means.
Seeing the squares as different colors is not an illusion. There is only one visual illusion in that example, and they don't remark on it: the illusion of seeing, in a flat surface, a 3D scene with light and shadow. The judgement that the two squares have different colors follows from that, because in the real-world scene the image depicts, those squares would in fact be surfaces with different colors when seen under the same light.
Re: (Score:1)
Also, what is the difference between seeing 3D in a 2D surface and seeing 3D in a 3D "surface"? Like the Flatlander that sees a penny as a line (he can only see one-dimensional information), everything we see is actually a 2D image translated into a 3D construction in our brain.
Re: (Score:2, Insightful)
No, it's not just a disagreement about terminology. It's a disagreement about what the object of vision is. I take it we all agree that what vision does is to give us information about the environment. The disagreement is about assumptions about what that information is. Is it information about the rays of light that hit the retina, or about the surfaces tha
Re: (Score:1)
Re: (Score:2)
Let me try it this way: perception is about acquiring information about the environment of an organism. The environment isn't just a 3D surface (in the mathematical sense you're using) that the organism sees; it's a world that the organism inhabits. The organism moves around the environment and interacts with its features and objects with its limbs, et
Re: (Score:2, Interesting)
I think you totally misunderstood what they were getting at, poorly chosen words. According to physics of relativity, "insideness vs outsideness" is an
Re: (Score:3, Insightful)
I don't think physicists have very much to tell us about psychology or philosophy. What they think is not completely irrelevant, but it is not imbued with the authority you pretend it should.
Re: (Score:2)
But that makes assumptions about the evolution of vision.
Re: (Score:2)
The challenge is to compare the *ink* colours (lightness, hue, whatever) in two sections of that picture. The other prompts in the picture are there solely to inhibit the capability to accurately make a comparison by producing an illusion of a shadow cast by a cylinder.
The fact that one cannot easily do this as with a flat checkerboard for example, is what makes it an illusion.
Re: (Score:1)
In fact, it seems to me that your "purpose" of vision fits quite well with their basic idea of "perception is only a representation of reality."
Re: (Score:1)
That's a fair point. I was definitely sloppy about that, in the interest of advancing other parts of the argument.
When I say "the point of vision," I don't mean that nature is teleological. "The point of vision" is the set of functional assumptions that the researchers make, and guide their investigation. My argument is that by accepting the idea that people's "failur
Re: (Score:1)
Independent of this illusion discussion, if you don't mind, could you tell me if there i
No really, it's an illusion. (Score:2)
Re: (Score:1)
This is why... (Score:5, Insightful)
> 3 (Score:1, Funny)
The pips on my six-sided die say otherwise.
Re: 3 (Score:2)
FTA: across different languages most characters take three strokes to write out. That's because, he says, three is the highest quantity a person's brain can perceive without resorting to counting.
If we wrote Morse code, yes. To my knowledge (which may be false) most writing systems distinguish symbols primarily by their shapes, not by the number of strokes. Then again, TFA may be misleading. From Changizi's own page [geocities.com] (gasp! a source reference!) "(1) The number of strokes per character is approximately three, independent of the number of characters in the writing system; numeral systems are the exception, having on average only two strokes per character. (2) Characters are approximately 50% redunda
I need a new brain... its malfunctioning... (Score:2)
After reading the caption and looking fo rthe illusion I was able to see more.
Re: (Score:2)
The number three... (Score:1)
Re: (Score:3, Interesting)
Re: (Score:1)
So... (Score:2)
Chok-sa!